Skip to main content

Thinking about the law like a psychological scientist

Before psychological scientists design studies and collect data, we need something to study. That is, we need questions to answer. For researchers like us, specialists in psychology and law*, these questions are usually related to the legal system. But where specifically do we get our ideas? In some cases, they are the result of applying our scientific knowledge and training to a topic; in others, we see an event or case and want to know more about it.

Kitty Genovese's murder inspired a program of social psychology research
One place we get our ideas is from real-life events, like criminal cases. Sometimes these are cases that are reported widely by the media; sometimes they are cases we encounter in the course of our work. Cases that seem to defy common sense make us ask questions about how they could have happened. A prominent example of a crime inspiring research is the infamous murder of Catherine “Kitty” Genovese. Kitty was murdered in 1964 in Queens, New York in a brutal attack that occurred over a time period of about half an hour. Press coverage of her murder suggested that several dozen eyewitnesses failed to intervene or call the police as she was attacked. Although it later emerged that this reporting about the number of eyewitnesses was incorrect**, the notion that large numbers of people could fail to intervene in an emergency sparked the interest of researchers John Darley and Bibb Latané.

It’s shocking that so many people could see an emergency unfolding and do nothing. Many social commentators at the time opined that modern society made people callous. But perhaps it was instead something inherent in the situation that made people less likely to intervene. Darley and Latané started a program of research on the conditions in which bystanders are more or less likely to intervene in emergencies or when others are in distress1. Much of this research comprises experiments in which participants – who are unaware of the true nature of the study – witness an urgent situation and must decide, without prompting, whether they will intervene and assist. The researchers can control many variables in the situation, including the number of bystanders, the nature of the emergency, and who is in distress. This research has found that, contrary what common sense might tell us, as the number of bystanders in the area increases, individual bystanders are less likely to intervene and a person in distress is less likely to receive assistance overall2.

We are also often compelled to conduct research by cases of wrongful conviction. Consider the case of Juan Rivera, who was wrongfully convicted of rape and murder in 1993, partly on the basis of a false confession which had been obtained after a lengthy and stressful interrogation***. Rivera had a strong alibi and DNA evidence recovered from the victim did not match him. But despite the exculpatory evidence, the prosecution charged Rivera and offered an implausible argument to explain the lack of a DNA match: that the 11-year old victim had consensual sex prior to being assaulted by Rivera, who must have failed to ejaculate (and failed to leave any physical trace at the scene). At three separate trials, Rivera was found guilty, and he spent a total of 19 years in prison before being exonerated.

How is that people can be convicted, even in the face of such strong exculpating evidence? This case and others like it**** inspired researchers Sara Appleby and Saul Kassin to conduct studies in which people acted as jurors in simulated cases where all of the evidence and facts were the same, save variations in how the attorneys argued the case. They found that improbable prosecutorial theories that attempt to explain exculpatory evidence, like the one used in Rivera’s trial, can be remarkably persuasive to jurors3. Put another way, a prosecutor suggesting a crazy story may very well tilt the trial against the defendant. This may be because people greatly try to understand complex information (like evidence in a trial) with coherent stories, and the prosecutorial theories, though vanishingly unlikely, provide a way of understanding the apparent inconsistencies in the evidence (e.g., a confession and DNA evidence that conflict).

Sensational and counter-intuitive cases compel important questions about how apparently extreme events can occur, but common legal and law enforcement practices can also inspire research, especially if they are based on untested assumptions or if we don’t understand them very well. For example, last week we wrote about our research project related to bait questions – a common interview technique in which an interrogator asks a suspect about hypothetical evidence (which may not actually exist). That project got its start a couple years ago when we shared an office at John Jay College as the result of a conversation during some downtime.

As Timothy remembers it*****, he was complaining about bait questions to some other colleagues in the office. He had recently run a training course for law enforcement and military interrogators, and he found that many of them repeatedly used bait questions in the training interrogations. His intuition was that something seemed problematic about them, but he struggled to clearly identify what it was. Overhearing the conversation, Will pointed out that bait questions sounded a lot like the way researchers suggest misinformation to participants in memory studies – a field of research that had never been applied to interrogations in this way before. We thought it might be possible that bait questions, despite being hypothetical, might lead people to believe the suggested evidence really existed, like a classic misinformation effect. We then put our heads together and planned the first of our studies on how bait questions could influence memory4.

Alternatively, we might draw from past research or psychological theory and try to apply it to issues in the legal system. That is, we might start with a practical problem in a legal context, and then we try to use what we already know from psychology to solve that problem. One of the most successful examples of researchers applying well-established psychology to a real-world problem is the Cognitive Interview. Witnesses and victims often have difficulty remembering details of what they experienced – after all, memory is rarely perfect. The Cognitive Interview was developed by Ron Fisher and Ed Gieselman, as a technique for enhancing witnesses’ memory5.

The Cognitive Interview comprises a series of interviewing techniques based on our theoretical understanding of how memory works. The ability of a person to remember something depends on at least two things: (1) the quality of the information stored in a person’s mind (what psychologists call a “memory trace”) and (2) the circumstances under which the person is trying to retrieve the memory (what we call the a quality of the “retrieval cues”). To understand what this means, imagine a memory is a printed photo, in one of several albums. The photo can be clear, or it can be fuzzy and faded (this is like the quality of the memory trace). If you’re looking for a specific picture, regardless of the quality of the photo, it can be easier to find it if you’re already looking in the right album (this is like the quality of the retrieval cues).

When we interview a witness, we don’t have control over how good their memory trace is. However, we can try to optimize the conditions of retrieval. From classic memory research we know that people can remember more information when they have already mentally accessed information related to what they’re trying to remember – that is, people can use related information to “cue” their memory. For instance, it’s easier to remember what the place you grew up was like if you catch a whiff of a familiar smell from your childhood (like freshly baked cookies or newly cut grass)6. Based on this idea, Cognitive Interview includes a technique called mental context reinstatement, which involves having the witness actively recall the sensory experience of the event – what they saw, heard, smelled, and felt to use as memory cues for other relevant information.  Dozens of experiments have demonstrated that this tool can assist witnesses in remembering substantially more details about what happened. In this way, our general knowledge of memory helps to solve a specific, applied problem in the justice system.

Beyond the ones we discussed here, there are plenty of other sources of inspiration for researchers. What all these sources of research ideas have in common is that they raise unanswered questions in psychology and law. Once we have a question, we can apply our scientific knowledge and training to design and conduct an experiment to find an empirical answer. Yet each experiment we conduct starts with a common core – curiosity. Anything that stokes curiosity can be the beginnings of research, whether a newspaper story, law article, or chapter in a psychology textbook. In this way, you do not need to have a PhD in order to do research. You just need to want to know more about the world around you.

This post was written by Timothy Luke and Will Crozier

* The intersection of psychology and law is also sometimes called legal psychology or forensic psychology. Unfortunately, these terms are used inconsistently, and there’s little agreement on what to call ourselves.

** Kitty Genovese’s case is too complicated to explore thoroughly here. For an interesting read about the complexity surrounding her murder and the subsequent investigation, see Kassin, S. M. (2017). The killing of Kitty Genovese: what else does this case tell us? Perspectives on psychological science12(3), 374-381.

*** “Stressful” is something of an understatement. After the interrogation, Rivera had an apparent psychotic episode and repeatedly slammed his head against a wall in jail, while the police typed a confession for him to sign.

**** In rape cases in which DNA recovered from the victim does not match the defendant, prosecutors often argue that the DNA must have originated from another subject – possibly another consensual sexual partner or someone else who participated in the rape but was not caught. These arguments are often highly improbable, yet they appear in numerous cases – so many that people have taken to calling it the “unindicted co-ejaculator” theory.

***** As a case study in how fickle memory can be, Will and Timothy can’t quite agree on the details of what he was complaining about.

References

[1] Darley, J. M., & Latané, B. (1968). Bystander intervention in emergencies: diffusion of responsibility. Journal of personality and social psychology8(4), 377-383.

[2] Fischer, P., Krueger, J. I., Greitemeyer, T., Vogrincic, C., Kastenmüller, A., Frey, D., ... & Kainbacher, M. (2011). The bystander-effect: a meta-analytic review on bystander intervention in dangerous and non-dangerous emergencies. Psychological bulletin137(4), 517-537.

[3] Appleby, S. C., & Kassin, S. M. (2016). When Self-report Trumps Science. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law22(2), 127-140.

[4] Luke, T. J., Crozier, W. E., & Strange, D. (2017). Memory errors in police interviews: The bait question as a source of misinformation. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition6(3), 260-273.

[5] Memon, A., Meissner, C. A., & Fraser, J. (2010). The Cognitive Interview: A meta-analytic review and study space analysis of the past 25 years. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law16(4), 340-372.

[6] Easterbrook, J. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological review66(3), 183-201.

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

Let's talk about the role of psychology in law

Will and Timothy are joined by guest Dr. Jason Chin, for a chat about the relationship between psychology and law. In this chat format, we gather regular authors and guests in Slack and have a moderated conversation, guided by prompts and questions selected in advance. Participants get to respond to each other's points, make comments, and ask each other questions in real-time. The transcript has been lightly edited. Will Crozier &#x1F419 Welcome to another Exercise in Exceptions chat! We’ve talked a lot about how psychology research can influence the law – but that research needs to make it into the courts to actually make the intended difference. However, it’s never as easy as explaining a study or two to a jury. In this chat, we’re going to discuss this collision a bit – how psychology science is used in the legal system. Timothy and I are joined by Dr. Jason Chin , a lecturer at the TC Beirne School of Law at University of Queensland, Australia. Wel

When investigations go wrong – in science and policework

A story of both a wrongful conviction and scientific fraud We’ve talked about many of the ways police investigations can go wrong, including mistaken eyewitness identifications , memory errors , and false confessions . Often, when people imagine police investigations running afoul, they imagine egregious cases in which police plant evidence or physically torture suspects to get them to produce confessions they know are false. Although situations like that do occur, mistakes in investigations require no intentional wrongdoing. A detective doesn’t need to be trying to get a false confession, for instance, in order to get one ( as our guest writer Fabi Alceste has written about) . Errors happen often without the investigators realizing anything has gone wrong. Similarly, when people imagine bad scientific research happening, they often imagine scientists fabricating data or committing outright fraud. Scientific fraud is a problem, but it’s quite rare. However, there are many questio

Interrogation, coercion, and false confessions: The case of Brendan Dassey

In December 2015, Netflix brought the justice system to the forefront of the public consciousness with its award-winning documentary Making a Murderer . The 10-episode serialization followed the story of Steven Avery – a Wisconsin man who was wrongfully convicted of rape in 1985, spent 18 years in prison before being exonerated, and was then arrested and ultimately convicted for the murder of Teresa Halbach in 2005. While the documentary focuses primarily on Steven Avery as the main suspect, Making a Murderer also highlighted a lesser-known element of the case: the confession and conviction of Brendan Dassey, Avery’s nephew. Although there’s a lot to analyze in this case, we focus here on a nagging question – did Brendan Dassey give a false confession? Dassey’s involvement in the case was little understood until Netflix’s deep dive into the case, but has emerged as one of the most scrutinized and tragic elements. Briefly, the police focused on Avery as a suspect in the disappe