The legal system is a source of endless
fascination. Crime novels, cop shows, procedural dramas – while fiction, all
depict the process by which society punishes the bad guys and protects the
innocent. Of course, we need not always turn to fiction to be pulled in by
compelling stories of crime and legal processes. Real life cases, reported on
by the news, true-crime shows, and increasingly deep-dive documentaries, books,
and podcasts can be as engaging as the fictional cases. There exist many parts
of the justice system that we are drawn to, spanning from how people can commit
crimes, to how crimes are investigated, to what happens in trial. But how much
do we, as society, truly understand the legal process we have devised to
protect and serve us? We often imagine the legal system is made up of rules,
regulations, procedures, and a dash of forensic science to make sure that justice
is fair. However, in many cases, the design of the legal system is based on
weak evidence and harmful misconceptions.
Take, for instance, the growing movement to
do away with cash bail. On its face, the bail system makes intuitive sense –
the risk of losing money will motivate people to show up for their trial. In
practice, however, bail leads to a system that punishes the poor. Those that
cannot afford thousands to tens of thousands of dollars don’t get to go home.
Instead, they remain in jail as legally innocent people, until they have been
convicted or acquitted in court.
This, of course, is but one example. In fact,
psychological scientists across the world have dedicated entire careers to
identifying problems and solutions in the justice system. This blog is run by
two such scientists. Dr. Timothy Luke and Dr. William Crozier both received
their PhDs in psychology from John Jay College of Criminal Justice, part of the
City University of New York*. Though we both study psychology and law, we have
different specialties. Timothy’s work has focused on police and military interrogation
techniques, confessions, and lie detection. Will’s work focuses on how memory,
and particularly memory mistakes, impact the justice system throughout the
legal process. In our research collaborations, we find examples of where social
and cognitive psychology interact in the law, identifying what problems may
arise, and how we can fix it.
Briefly, we use scientific inquiry (for
example, experiments with human participants) to identify ways in which legal
practices may not be as fair as they should be. Equipped with scientific
evidence, we take what we learn and try to create new practices for
investigations and legal procedures that will not put innocent people at risk
for wrongful conviction, while at the same time identifying and bringing the
real culprits to justice.
Unfortunately, most psychological research
that relates to the legal system is only reported in academic journals, in
highly technical language. Or worse, it is translated to the public by
non-academic publications that can exaggerate or misinterpret the science. If
you’ve ever wanted scientific answers about how memory works, how to tell if
someone is lying, how to conduct an effective interrogation, or whether juries
can disregard inadmissible evidence – we’ll talk about those things, and more..
If you binged Making a Murderer or Serial, wonder what Criminal Minds gets right and gets wrong, or always react to the dun duun of Law and Order, then this is the place for you Here on this blog, we
plan to explore research at the intersection of psychology and law in a style
and in formats accessible to people less familiar with scientific reporting. We
will approach the legal system from a scientific standpoint, specifically
focusing on psychological processes at work in the justice system.
We’ll discuss both current and classic
psychology research that shows us legal practices don’t work the way we naively
expect them to – like that people will confess to a crime they didn’t commit.
We’ll also discuss things we don’t know and need more research on, such as the
best ways to interrogate suspects and informants to obtain information.
Occasionally, we may get a bit sidetracked by talking about the difference between
good science and bad science, or the type of things psychologists can, and
cannot, conclude from their work. Overall, we’ll be discussing the things that
fascinate us about the legal system, including real-life criminal cases, controversial
policies, and implementations of science-based practices.
As our blog title suggests, an overarching
theme will be finding exceptions to the rules we often take for granted. The
legal system is, to a large extent, built on assumed rules about the way people
behave and the way certain legal practices should work. But psychological
science tells us these assumed rules often have important exceptions. A
detailed confession is an excellent piece of evidence – except when it’s given
by an innocent person. Several witnesses giving consistent statements about how
a crime happened are highly informative – except when they’ve been speaking to
each other and have contaminated each other’s memories. Plea bargains make the
criminal justice system operate more efficiently and effectively – except when
innocent people plead guilty. Here, we will often take a concept, idea, or
process (for example, eyewitness identification), discuss its intended purpose
(evidence to catch a perpetrator), and then find ways in which that concept,
idea, or process fails to accomplish its goal, like when a very confident
eyewitness actually makes a mistake about who they recognize and inadvertently sends
an innocent person to prison.
We’ll identify questions, and try to
determine if we have the scientific evidence to answer the question. If we
don’t have a good answer, we’ll try to figure out what it would take to find
the answer. We’ll argue with ourselves and others. We’ll debate and deliberate
about what we know about psychology and law and how we know it. We’ll question
rules, laws, and assumptions. This is an exercise in exceptions.
*Strictly speaking, our PhDs are from the
Graduate Center of CUNY, but we spent the vast majority of our grad school experience
working at John Jay.
Will Crozier and Timothy Luke wrote this post.
Will Crozier and Timothy Luke wrote this post.
Comments
Post a Comment