Bad news:
the police have brought you in for questioning in a recent murder. The good
news, though, is that you didn’t commit the murder! So all you have to do to
clear your name is give the police your alibi, right?
Not so
fast. Can you remember exactly where you were and what you were doing when the
crime was committed? What can you give the police to prove you’re telling the
truth? Are your alibi and the evidence supporting it going to be able to
convince someone on a jury? Actually, this is going to be more difficult than
you thought.
Using an
alibi to defend yourself is based on at least two different psychological elements;
first, you have to rely on your memory to construct a story of what you were
doing. Second, that story has to be believable. That is, you need to supply
proof that will convince other people. If you fail at either of these steps,
such as by misremembering or not remembering what you were doing or by relying
only on a family member to corroborate your story, your alibi probably won’t
help you much, if at all.
Try this:
Can you remember exactly what you were doing at 1pm a week ago? What about two
weeks ago? If you find this a very difficult task, you’re not alone – many of
us would struggle to remember what we did several days ago, let alone a longer
period. Research on memory backs this up. Unlike a video recorder, our memory
is not a running storage of what we’ve experienced. Instead, we only remember
some of the information, and even that can decay out of memory very quickly.
This means that when we try to remember something, we may only remember a few
of the main points and fill in many details with what makes sense, rather than
what we specifically remember1,2,3.
Meanwhile,
intuition suggests that if the stakes are high, such as when you’re trying to
prove to the police that you’re innocent, our memory can work better4.
However, if we never fully formed a memory for the alibi event, no amount of
focus or effort can make us remember. Further, even if we do form the memory
(called “encoding”), we may still fail to recall the memory, or “retrieve” it.
You may fail to remember more than few details about what you were doing, or
worse, recall the wrong memory. You may remember the wrong day, or you may
incorporate incorrect details into the memory – such as someone else’s
description of what happened.
Psychology
research investigating how people generate alibis demonstrates just how
difficult coming up with an alibi can be. In one study, researchers asked
participants to remember what they were doing three weeks earlier –
essentially, generate an alibi. The participants then spent a few days trying
to validate their own alibi and collect evidence to support it, before
generating a second alibi. When the researchers compared the first and second
alibis, they found the two statements on agreed on about 50% of the details5.
In a similar study, 36% of the participants changed their alibi after trying to
fact-check their first account6. These studies demonstrate that even
something as simple as trying to remember what you were doing at a previous
time and day can be pretty difficult – and that your first attempt will likely
contain many inaccuracies.
Many people
believe that changes in a story or telling a story out of order is a sign of
lying. In fact, under many circumstances, inconsistencies can be more common in
true stories than in lies because we make small memory mistakes in recounting
what happened. This means that it’s very difficult to remember a specific event
without making some mistakes7. And if you do make mistakes, the
police may think you’re lying to them – obviously not a good position to be in
if you’re trying to convince them you didn’t commit a crime.
However,
let’s say you have an amazing memory, or get lucky and do accurately remember
what happens, your alibi needs to be one that the police can investigate. Can
you provide evidence of what you were doing, such as security camera footage or
store receipts? This type of evidence would be the most helpful, but may not be
available or accessible.
Just because you're concentrating doesn't mean you're remembering better |
Is there
anyone who can confirm what you’re saying? Our friends and family are the
people we spend the most time with, and are likely the people we’d go to
support our alibi. However, people are inherently skeptical of your friends and
family – after all, they may be motivated to lie to protect you. Unmotivated
strangers, such as the barista that poured your coffee, or an unmotivated
familiar person, such as the security guard you see every day at work, would be
best. Indeed, when participants are given proof of an alibi that differs only
in who is supporting (and all the other facts of the case are the same), they
find alibis supported by an unmotivated stranger or familiar is much more
believable than a family member8. That of course presents yet
another challenge: will they remember you, and will they vouch for you?
Finally,
how believable is your alibi? Even if you can collect evidence to support your
alibi, you may still run into problems getting the police (or a jury, if you
end up on trial) to believe you. For one, both ordinary people and police
officers are automatically suspicious of alibis so you’re already fighting an
up-hill battle8,9. Second, not all evidence is created equal.
Evidence that is identifying and difficult to fake, such as security footage,
would be very convincing10. However, as mentioned, evidence such as
a friend vouching for you is less convincing.
Evidence
that would be easy to fake will also be helpful, but not completely convincing.
For example, you provide a movie ticket stub for the time in question. At
first, this seems helpful – how could you have committed the murder if you were
at the movies? But how do we know you didn’t get the ticket from someone else?
Or buy the ticket the day before and tear it yourself? Such evidence can
certainly help your case – but it’s not going to get you off the hook itself.
A good time, but not necessarily a good alibi. |
While the
psychology research on underlying memory and social psychology discussed above
paints a bit of a bleak picture for alibis, there’s still a lot we don’t
understand. For example, we know from research on memory, particularly in
educational-oriented areas, that how you ask a question can influence what
people are able to remember. In the alibi context, this suggests that how
police ask questions can increase memory for an alibi. In fact, one of our
current projects is looking at this question exactly*.
A primary
challenge to such research is finding strategies that increase the
opportunities for innocent suspects to prove their innocence without allowing
guilty suspects a chance to construct a lie. One possible strategy for
increasing alibi effectiveness would be to allow the suspect to check their own
alibi before the police investigate it. For innocent suspects this could be
very helpful by allowing them fill in blanks or ask their friends and family
for additional information. However at the same time, it would likely benefit
guilty suspects as well, allowing them to see if their fake alibi checks out,
and changing it if something doesn’t add up.
One
promising line of research11 is based on our understanding of how
memory retrieval works, which involves promoting the use of more “memory cues.”
Have you ever struggled to remember something, and as soon as you think of
something related (such as where you were, what you were doing, or someone’s
reaction) you suddenly recall it? Or ever noticed how easily you can remember
events when you’re in the place where it happened? These are all examples of
cues – memories that are tied to other memories, and can help jog our memory.
In the
alibi context, this means that if we can get people to use cues, either by
helping to generate them through questioning or suggesting a person
intentionally try to use them, it may lead to more accurate alibis. This
strategy would likely help innocent suspects, who are trying to draw on real
memories, while providing little benefit to guilty suspects who are not
accessing memories but fabricating a story instead. Of course, this idea still needs to be put to the test in a research lab.
Although
psychology research on alibis is relatively new and little, there is a lot of
research on the underlying aspects of alibi generation and evaluation. This
means that we have a lot of theory on how memory works and how jurors make
decisions on which to base future research. We are hopeful that the justice system
will consider such current and future research, and adjust policy such that
alibis can effectively protect the innocent.
This post was written by Will Crozier / edited by Timothy Luke.
Notes
* To see
the details of the project, including our pre-registration, go to osf.io/7nqpc
References
[1]
Bartlett, F. C., & Burt, C. (1933). Remembering: A study in experimental
and social psychology. British Journal of
Educational Psychology, 3(2), 187–192.
doi:10.1111/j.2044-8279.1933.tb02913.x
[2] Loftus,
E. F. (1996). Memory distortion and false memory creation. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Online,
24(3), 281–295.
[3]
Neisser, U. (1982). Memory Observed:
Remembering in Natural Contexts. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.
[4] Kassam,
K. S., Gilbert, D. T., Swencionis, J. K., & Wilson, T. D. (2009).
Misconceptions of memory the Scooter Libby effect. Psychological Science, 20(5), 551–552.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02334.x.
[5] Strange, D., Dysart, J., & Loftus, E. F. (2014). “Oops, I guess I made a mistake”:
Why alibi errors are not necessarily evidence of guilt. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 222, 82–90.
doi:10.1027/2151-2604/a000169.
[6] Olson,
E. A., & Charman, S. D. (2012). ‘But can you prove it?’ – examining the
quality of innocent suspects’ alibis. Psychology,
Crime, & Law, 18(5), 453–471. doi:10.1080/1068316X.2010.505567.
[7] DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. J.,
Malone, B. E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K., & Cooper, H. (2003). Cues to
Deception. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 74-118.
[8] Culhane, S. E., Hosch, H. M., &
Kehn, A. (2008a). Alibi generation: Data from US Hispanics and US non-Hispanic
whites. Journal of Ethnicity in Criminal
Justice, 6(3), 177–199. doi:10.1080/15377930802243395.
[9] Dysart, J., & Strange, D. S.
(2012). Beliefs about alibis and alibi investigations: A survey of law
enforcement. Psychology, Crime, &
Law, 18(1), 11–25. doi:10.1080/1068316X.2011.562867.
[10] Olson, E. A., & Wells, G. L.
(2004). What makes a good alibi? A proposed taxonomy. Law and Human Behavior, 28(2), 157–176.
doi:10.1023/B:LAHU.0000022320.47112.d3.
[11] Leins, D. A., & Charman, S. D.
(2016). Schema reliance and innocent alibi generation. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 21, 111–126.
doi:10.1111/lcrp.12035.
Comments
Post a Comment