In this chat format, we gather regular authors and guests in Slack and have a moderated conversation, guided by prompts and questions selected in advance. Participants get to respond to each other's points, make comments, and ask each other questions in real-time.
The transcript has been lightly edited.
rabbitsnore (Timothy J. Luke)
Welcome to another Exercise in Exceptions chat! This time,
our topic is the conference we attended recently -- that is, the meeting of the
American Psychology-Law Society (APLS). In a previous chat, we’ve talked with
Gabi Rico about her first conference experience at APLS. Today, we’re chatting
with a fellow researcher about our impressions of the content of the
conference.
Will and I are joined today by our friend Natalie Gordon.
Natalie, care to introduce yourself and say a little bit
about what kind of work you do?
Natalie Gordon
Sure! Thank you for having me. I am a PhD student in
psychology and law at John Jay College of Criminal Justice. My research is centered around decision-making—more
specifically, how jurors make decisions about punishment in capital cases and
damage awards in civil cases, and how long-term consequences might impact
defendants’ plea decisions.
rabbitsnore
Thanks for joining us, Natalie! We're glad to have you here.
Will Crozier
🐙
Indeed! Thanks for stopping by
rabbitsnore
Conferences are full of presentations -- posters, talks,
symposia. We all attended a bunch of these presentations, so let's start with a
sort of obvious question: What was your favorite presentation, in any format?
And of course, why?
Natalie, as our guest, you can take the first stab at this.
Natalie Gordon
If I may, I’d actually like to answer this question by first
providing you with the “why”, or what makes a talk, poster, or symposium my
favorite. It mainly has to do with inspiration. This can be inspiration from
someone’s presentation abilities or slides, or inspiration in the form of a new
research idea.
At AP-LS this year, I had two “favorites,” or sources of inspiration.
I was inspired by the symposium the two of you were a part of on “Why Memory
Matters.” I was impressed by how each of the five of you had captivated your
audience not simply through interesting research, but through your style of
presentation. Minimal text, lots of visuals, and, frequently, an anecdote at
the beginning to hook listeners in (e.g., the stories of Adnan Syed and Brendan
Dassey). Just as a teacher or professor in school can make or break students’
ability to learn, I think that how researchers present their data to audiences
can make or break whether the audience leaves feeling inspired and whether they
will even remember the research!
rabbitsnore
Aw, thanks for that!
Will Crozier
🐙
shucks!
🙃
rabbitsnore
The style of our presentations (in addition to the
substance) was something we all put a lot of effort into, so I'm glad it paid
off!
Natalie Gordon
It was really great! You have only yourselves to thank.
My second source of inspiration came from another symposium
I attended titled, “A system of pleas.”1 As I mentioned earlier, one of my
research interests is on how long-term (or “collateral”) consequences might
impact a defendant’s willingness to plead guilty to a crime. During this
symposium, Dr. Tina Zottoli presented research from a national survey of
policies and procedures related to the guilty plea process and one of their
findings was that fewer than half of U.S. states require the presentation of
what are called “collateral consequences instructions,” which provide
information on collateral consequences to defendants during plea bargaining.
Although relevant to my area of research, this actually was not what inspired
me about her talk. I was inspired by a comment she made towards the end of her
presentation when she posed the question, “How might collateral consequences
impact perceptions of procedural fairness?” Then, another presenter asked the
related question “How might offers framed in terms of fairness impact plea
decisions?” (to be honest, I may have thought of this question on my own during
the talk but I can’t remember; insert source monitoring nerd joke here).
Anyway, I immediately started plotting research studies to examine these issues
and, well, that’s where I am now. Inspired.
rabbitsnore
What do you mean by an offer framed in terms of fairness?
Can you say more about that?
Natalie Gordon
Sure, so by that I mean whether the attorney tells his/her
client that the deal is better than what most defendants would get, or that
it's on par with what others would get.
rabbitsnore
Interesting. I imagine that's the sort of advice attorneys
regularly give their clients.
Will Crozier
🐙
Are there legal restraints on that?
How would a defense attorney actually know what other offers
are made outside of their own experience, and could that lead to bad legal
advice?
Natalie Gordon
I think it definitely could lead to bad legal advice, and
it's unclear whether they would actually be making such a statement because
they believe it's true or because they want to get their client to plead
guilty.
But I think that in studies of plea bargaining, it would be
important to look at how the kind of language the attorneys use impacts
defendants' willingness to plea. So whether they frame the offer as one that is
fair or other tools of persuasion they might be able to use.
Will Crozier
🐙
Sounds like you've got your dissertation topic! haha
Natalie Gordon
Haha, it's a possibility!
rabbitsnore
Will, what about you? Favorite presentation?
Will Crozier
🐙
I'm going to have to give a bit of a cop out answer and
reference a whole session that I enjoyed
rabbitsnore
If you say your favorite was our symposium I'm kicking you
out of this chat.
Natalie Gordon
Hahaha!
Will Crozier
🐙
I think ranking the symposiums I've organized is the subject
for another chat!
But, I went to a bright-and-early-8-am talk Saturday
morning. It was “eyewitness memory” session but wasn’t explicitly eyewitness
memory...more like memory applied to eyewitness situations. A really good
friend of mine, Rob Michael, presented about how the ordering of hard-and-easy
eyewitness questions might affect test performance or confidence.2 Another talk
on how to elicit specific types of information during questioning, such as
episodic info or semantic info.3 The third talk discussed how emotionality might
affect what central or peripheral information people pick up.4 The fourth was
about a form of context reinstatement to perhaps improve eyewitness memory5, and
the last talk discussed how taking a test right after an event can actually
make you more susceptible to memory distortion and misinformation.6
rabbitsnore
Props for going to an 8am session on the last day of the
conference.
Natalie Gordon
That sounds interesting! More motivation to wake up early...
rabbitsnore
Why was that your favorite?
Will Crozier
🐙
All of these talks had some good memory theory behind them,
and were applied to the eyewitness context (although not EXCLUSIVELY
eyewitness). The work used a variety of methodologies, but made some
interesting points about eyewitness memory that, I think, sometimes gets lost
in a conference as super-applied as APLS.
Plus, I pulled out some nuggets for my own work
For example, for our study on alibis: the talk about how to
elicit different types of information was helpful, because we may want to ask
people about specific pieces of information to include in their alibis in a way
that enhances memory recall
So if we wanted to know what someone was doing at a specific
time, we would want to elicit more autobiographical information. In our study,
we also use a strategy called "Mental Context Reinstatement," and one
of the talks was about a variation of this strategy called meditative context
reinstatement. For that example, the research wasn't on evoking alibi memories
per se, but still might be something we look into for future research.
Natalie Gordon
Could you remind me what mental context reinstatement is?
Will Crozier
🐙
Sure!
In it's most simple form, you ask someone to close their
eyes, and try to reimagine a specific memory. In the context of alibi
generation, it would be something like "Think about what you were doing
yesterday at 1pm. Close your eyes and try to focus on who you were with...where
you were...what you were doing...try to imagine these details."
In theory, it generates more cues to help you reconstruct
the memory than, say, just asking, "Hey what were you doing yesterday at 1
pm?"
Natalie Gordon
Ah okay. So can you also explain what you mean when you say
that you would want to ask people about specific pieces of information to
include in their alibis?
Will Crozier
🐙
If we were trying to evoke certain details, such as if we
really wanted to know who someone was with. So if the police wanted to find
someone to verify your alibi, they might want you to remember who you were with.
Whereas some other detail, like exactly what you were doing, might be less
relevant - relevant, but perhaps not the most important piece of information
you could remember. But that's why I really enjoyed the session - none of the
talks were specifically about these issues, but I was able to get some good
ideas out of them.
Natalie Gordon
I see! That's great.
Will Crozier
🐙
Timothy, how about you - favorite presentation?
rabbitsnore
Similar to what Natalie said earlier -- my favorite talk of
any conference is usually one that gets me thinking about something I
previously hadn’t considered. In this case, it was Kevin Colwell’s talk about
innocent suspects lying to the police.7 There are a lot of instances where
innocent people lie to the police. Perhaps they committed some other crime or
transgression and want to conceal it. Perhaps they don’t trust the police. This
probably happens all the time, but we haven’t really studied it in the
controlled environment of a psychology laboratory – or at all, as far as I
know. The talk and the methods of the study he presented weren’t theoretically
groundbreaking, but it brought light to an issue that I think we’ve largely
ignored in the deception literature so far.
In fact, most of what Dr. Colwell discussed to frame his
talk wasn't deception research at all. Rather, he talked about real cases he
had consulted on as an expert witness, in which he believed (and in some cases
they could prove) that an innocent person had lied to the police.
In my own work, I've noticed a few occurrences of innocent
people lying in studies where I've had people committing mock crimes and then
being interviewed. I've never had a good explanation for the behavior (and
honestly, I haven't thought about it very much), and I'm really glad someone is
working on this problem.
Will Crozier
🐙
Is that the first study you've seen on the topic?
rabbitsnore
Literally the first I have ever seen. And to put it in
context, the deception literature has been around since the late 1800s.
Natalie Gordon
I mentioned procedural justice earlier and I think that's
relevant here too. I believe there is research showing that increasing perceptions
of procedural justice (or fairness) in community members increases trust in
police.
So one reason innocents might lie to police is because they
think the process that got them there was unfair, or their treatment was
unfair, etc.
rabbitsnore
Yeah, I've had similar thoughts!
In fact, one thing I noticed in the handful of innocents who
lied to the interviewers in my lab is that they were all people of color. It’s
possible people who are often marginalized in society have lower trust in the
authorities. I don't have the data to give anything even close to a conclusive
answer for innocent lying, but procedural fairness issues have crossed my mind
as well.
Will Crozier
🐙
We recently used a scale to measure perceptions of
procedural fairness in a paper on body worn cameras that just got published in Psychology,
Public Policy, and Law. Perhaps that would be of interest to ya'lls' future
studies
rabbitsnore
Oh, nice. Yes, please.
Natalie Gordon
Please do pass it along!
rabbitsnore
Aside from our favorite talks, did you two notice any new
trends at the conference? Natalie, let's start with you again.
Natalie Gordon
What I notice is, of course, biased because I go to AP-LS
looking for sessions on research that I care about (with a dash of research
that I just find interesting), so my focus this year was on juries and plea
bargaining. But one thing I did notice was that greater attention is being
given to research on plea bargaining. This year there were three sessions
devoted entirely to plea bargaining (one on juveniles and guilty pleas, the one
on a “system of pleas” that I mentioned earlier, and one just titled “Pleas”),
whereas last year there were no sessions on plea bargaining and the year before
that there was only one session on plea bargaining. I’m happy that more
attention is being given to plea bargaining at AP-LS because we are a system of
pleas and, as such, deserves more of our attention. It’s also an area that is
ripe for research.
Will Crozier
🐙
...man, that was going to be my answer, too
Natalie Gordon
Great minds...! 🙂
rabbitsnore
Side note, when I saw the sign for the "Pleas"
session, I first read it as a misspelling of "Please."
And I was so confused.
Natalie Gordon
Hahaha
rabbitsnore
Is it your sense that the new push toward plea bargaining
research is here to stay?
Natalie Gordon
I would say so. There was a special issue in Psych, Public
Policy, & Law on plea bargaining (actually I think it's forthcoming).
I think there will be more of that. And I certainly hope
there is!
rabbitsnore
Given that well over 90% of cases in the US are resolved
with a guilty plea, the topic should definitely receive more attention that it
has.
rabbitsnore
As the one person in this chat not in the US, I feel
compelled to point out that not every system is a system of pleas, though.
Sweden, where I'm living and working right now, doesn't have
guilty pleas at all. Every case that is prosecuted goes to trial. But there's a
different research problem: Sweden uses panels of professional and lay judges,
rather than juries. We know very little about decision-making in these mixed tribunals.
Natalie Gordon
We're just starting up a study on mixed tribunals in my lab. I'll have
to report back to you!
rabbitsnore
Really? That's awesome!
Will Crozier 🐙
I also noticed that there was still a bit of debate over
exactly how to best think about eyewitness lineups.
There was some of that last year as well...I believe Gary
Wells pointed out that it's interesting we have an idea of what makes a lineup unfair
to a suspect, but we're still trying to work out how to make lineups fair, but
still be useful to police
rabbitsnore
Is there a perception that fair lineups are less useful?
Will Crozier
🐙
Well, if you make a lineup a really, really, really hard
memory test, for example by making all of the people in the lineups look almost
identical, then most people probably won't have a strong enough memory to pick
the "right" person. So on one hand if a lineup is unfair, the suspect
really jumps out at you because they look too different from everyone
else...like, he's the only person with a beard. On the other hand, if everyone
looks almost identical, it's basically guessing who the person you remember is,
because memory for faces is pretty hard
rabbitsnore
Right, there's almost always a tradeoff between hit rate and
false positives.
Will Crozier
🐙
Any trends you noticed, Timothy?
rabbitsnore
I’m going to answer this question by breaking the rules a
bit and talking about a trend that I’ve noticed for a while. For a several
years, I’ve been disappointed by how little theory has been discussed in
deception research. A lot of deception research – especially deception
detection research – is really lacking in theoretical grounding. Much of it is
focused on practicality (e.g., does accuracy increase or decrease in
such-and-such a situation?). I understand that the conference is, of course,
largely focused on practical issues, but I wish people would get back to the
social and cognitive psychological roots of deception more. I think it would
help researchers generate more thoughtful and interesting ideas.
Will Crozier
🐙
Theory is something that sometimes gets skimmed over in
talks in general, I think. It's usually a "based on such and such a
theory, we'd see this..." without, perhaps, an in-depth look at why. And
now that you mention it, i struggle to remember how often theories are
tested...which can be tough at an applied conference like APLS, but that
doesn't make it less important scientifically
Natalie Gordon
I agree. I think it's important for there to be application
of theory in psycho-legal research.
rabbitsnore
Speaking of things the field needs, let's move on to our
last question: Was there anything at the conference you wanted to see more of
or that you thought could be improved?
Natalie Gordon
Yes to both! One area of research that is underrepresented
at AP-LS is in the civil arena. There was only one session at the conference
this year devoted to this kind of research. So much of what is presented at
AP-LS focuses on the criminal side of our justice system, but there is also a
civil side with a range of case types—personal injury, medical malpractice,
patents, etc.—and it would be interesting to see more research there. This also
relates to what I think could be improved at AP-LS: a greater attorney
presence. Few attorneys attend AP-LS, but that means that the questions asked
of our panelists and the opportunities to be provided with insight by those at
the frontlines of the criminal justice system are limited. I think efforts need
to be made to recruit attorneys who can come speak to academics in a
constructive and potentially even collaborative way.
Will Crozier
🐙
Both excellent points
rabbitsnore
Do you have any ideas about why there aren't more lawyers at
the conference?
Natalie Gordon
I would imagine part of it is that they just don't know
about it, but I also wonder whether they would come even if they did. I think
some would, those who think speaking to psychologists is important and who
respect the research that we do. I think others wouldn't, especially if they
think what we do in the laboratory is not relevant to actual cases, and it's
that group I think we especially need to talk to so that we can have the
opportunity to explain the importance and application of our research.
Will Crozier
🐙
I wonder to what extent the perception of APLS is that we're
a bunch of out-of-touch psychologists just looking to ruin law enforcement
(and by "I wonder to what extent" I mean, I really
don't know)
rabbitsnore
There are a fair number of law enforcement officers who
attend the conference, but like with lawyers, probably not as many as there
should be.
To some extent, I worry that if more lawyers and police attended,
they'd be lost in all the research-babble. Our presentations are often full of
jargon and might be inaccessible.
Natalie Gordon
True, but this might motivate us to become better at
communicating to that audience so that we might change any negative perceptions
that exist or prevent confusion.
rabbitsnore
I totally agree.
Will, what about you? Anything you wanted more of or would
like to see improved?
Will Crozier
🐙
Hm. 8 am is too early
rabbitsnore
Heh.
Will Crozier
🐙
I did go on a twitter rant about wanting to see more about
open and reproducible research. So I’ll save you from rehashing that, except to
say: hopefully there’s more mention of preregistration, open materials, and
open data at next year’s conference.
It's a really important part of science that is particularly
valuable to psychology...and it seems like perhaps it hasn't quite caught on
yet at APLS
Natalie Gordon
Maybe there's an idea for a pre-conference workshop?
rabbitsnore
That was my biggest area of concern, too. I hardly saw any
open science, apart from the work we were involved in.
A workshop might be a good idea!
The official APLS journal -- Law and Human Behavior -- has
been making increasing efforts to support open science in their publications.
But it would be great if there were more support for it at the conference.
Will Crozier
🐙
I do think there's some room for some official endorsement, like
pre-conference workshops, or even symposiums
But I think cultural endorsement would be really helpful and
powerful too...if everyone/most people/some people publicize their open science
practices, i think it will catch on
Natalie Gordon
It's important. I know that my lab wants to start working on
pre-registering all of our projects.
Will Crozier
🐙
Exactly! Grass-roots open science campaigns
rabbitsnore
I just did a talk on open science for my research group here
at the University of Gothenburg. I have a bunch of resources I can pass along,
if they'd be helpful.
Will Crozier 🐙
Your lab starts doing it, you mention it to researchers
elsewhere, they start doing it...basically, open science going viral
Apart from open science, I'd say I really enjoyed the data
blitzes
I think they're a good practice, and a great way to fit a
few more talks in to the conference schedule
I hope they bring them back next year
Natalie Gordon
I was curious about those! I didn't get to attend any. Glad
to hear you liked them, I thought it was a neat idea.
rabbitsnore
We touched on this in our chat with Gabi... I actually
didn't like them very much.
Will Crozier
🐙
Oh?
rabbitsnore
Don't get me wrong. I really like the format, and I could
pick out individual talks I thought were great.
But I thought the sessions were a bit too loosely organized.
Will Crozier
🐙
So an execution problem?
rabbitsnore
There was no unifying theme at the sessions, so you could
easily end up listening to a talk about a topic you know very little about. When
you only have a few minutes to give a talk, it's hard to present your ideas in
a way that someone unfamiliar with the topic can figure out what's going on. It's
a challenge for the presenters, and it's a challenge for the audience.
So I'd prefer to see more thematic organization. But I'm
fussing about the details of an otherwise good idea.
Natalie Gordon
That seems like a fair point and something they could easily
fix.
Will Crozier
🐙
Gotta find the anonymous suggestion box
rabbitsnore
I hope so! Like I said, I really enjoy the format.
We've worked through all the questions I have. Any final
comments before we wrap up?
Will Crozier
🐙
I don't think I got a chance to specifically plug for
Natalie's talk, but it was quite good!
I'm looking forward to seeing your next presentation
Natalie Gordon
Aw well thank you! I appreciate that.
I was actually going to say that this was my favorite AP-LS
so far. I've been to five now (I think), and I'm glad I got to speak about my
thoughts on AP-LS this year with the two of you. You learn a lot about how to
make the conference fruitful for you with every year that you go.
rabbitsnore
Having been to a lot of conferences myself -- I heartily
agree.
Thank you again for joining us and sharing your thoughts,
Natalie!
Natalie Gordon
Thank you for having me! It's been a pleasure.
References
[1]State of the States: Advancing Guilty Plea Research Through a National Survey of United States Law
Tina M. Zottoli, PhD - Montclair State University; Tarika Daftary-Kapur, PhD - Fairleigh Dickinson University; Vanessa A. Edkins, PhD - Florida Institute of Technology; Allison D. Redlich, PhD - George Mason University; Christopher M. King, JD, PhD - Montclair State University; Lucian E. Dervan, JD - Belmont University; Elizabeth Tahan, BA/BS - Montclair State University
Abstract: We conducted a national survey of statutes, case law, and court rules in the United States pertaining to the guilty plea process. We present the results of our survey in three areas (time-limited offers, collateral consequences instructions and baseless pleas) and we discuss implications for future research that is informed by jurisdictional differences in policies and procedures across the U.S. We anticipate that these data will facilitate the development of novel research questions about the impact of existing policies on plea decisionmaking and outcomes.
[2] The Order of Question Difficulty Affects Eyewitness Beliefs: A Meta-Analysis of Two Paradigms Robert B. Michael, PhD - University of Louisiana at Lafayette; Yana Weinstein, PhD - University of Massachusetts Lowell
Abstract: A simple change to test construction—flipping questions from an easy-to-hard to hard-to-easy order— affects test performance evaluations. This effect has been demonstrated in eyewitnesses and students. But with limited studies, it is difficult to know a precise estimate of the effect’s magnitude. We meta-analyzed 7,215 participants across 39 studies to more precisely estimate the influence of question difficulty order. These more precise and robust estimates show that question difficulty order affects performance evaluations: Easy-to-hard questions produce 8% more optimistic performance evaluations than hard-to-easy questions. These more precise effect size estimates have implications for test constructors and interviewers.
[3] Memory and the Cooperative Source: How Can We Obtain Richer Information?
Jacob M. Champagne, BA/BS - University of Michigan - Flint; Haley Dawson, BA/BS - University of Michigan - Flint; Peter F. Molinaro, PhD - University of Michigan - Flint
Abstract: Investigators need to gather rich information about suspects, from others, connected to that individual. While current interviewing research focuses on cooperative witnesses, little research has examined interviewing strategies for cooperative sources. Cooperative sources rely on both semantic and episodic memories when reporting their “biographical memories”. Biographical memories may be organized within autobiographical memory, and thus, be subject to organizational mnemonics. The current experiment tested two mnemonics: semantic categorization and life period retrieval strategies. Amounts of information gathered in both conditions were compared to a control condition, where no retrieval/organizational strategy was implemented. Results and implications for investigative interviewing are discussed.
[4] Emotional Memory: The Role of Personality Traits and the Central-Peripheral Trade-Off
Lauren Thompson, BA/BS - Carleton University; Ellen Tansony, MA/MS - Carleton University; Adelle Forth, PhD - Carleton University
Abstract: With a sample of 68 students, the current study sought to explore the effects of the Five-Factor Model personality dimensions and central-peripheral tradeoffs on emotional memory and attention. Participant’s eye movements were tracked as they viewed a negative, positive, and neutral image. Memory for these images was subsequently tested. Results indicated that memory was enhanced for the positive compared to the negative image and a central-peripheral trade-off was found only for the positive and neutral images. When personality 114 Saturday, March 10th was assessed, neuroticism and openness were both positively related to memory of the negative image. Eye tracking patterns were unrelated to memory.
[5] Meditative Context Reinstatement on Eyewitness Metacognition
Brittany Race, MA/MS - University of Arkansas; James M. Lampinen, PhD - University of Arkansas
Abstract: Participants watched a video featuring a car theft. They were then given a target-present or target-absent, sixperson lineup. Following their choice they were given
either confirming or neutral feedback. Following feedback participants either completed a filler task only or a filler task and a guided context reinstatement, focusing on when they made a decision in the lineup. Participants then completed post-identification feedback questions. Context reinstatement provided a more diagnostic value to confidence statements. Correct choosers reported significantly increased levels of confidence with context reinstatement, but incorrect choosers showed no significant effect of context reinstatement.
[6] Does Retrieval Enhance Eyewitness Suggestibility Because Taking an Initial Memory Test Increases the Perceived Accuracy of the Misinformation Narrative?
Krista D. Manley, MA/MS - Iowa State University; Jason CK. Chan, PhD - Iowa State University
Abstract: Despite the robust memory-enhancing benefits of retrieval practice, it can ironically exacerbate eyewitness’ susceptibility to later misinformation - a phenomenon known as Retrieval-Enhanced Suggestibility (RES; Chan, Manley, & Lang, 2017). One explanation for this finding is that after taking a memory test, participant witnesses are more likely to treat the subsequently-presented misinformation narrative as providing corrective feedback (e.g., Rindal, DeFranco, Rich, & Zaragoza, 2016). In the current study, we provided a direct test of this hypothesis by asking participants to rate their perceived accuracy of the narrative. Our results are inconsistent with this account of RES.
[7]Lies by the Innocent
Kevin Colwell, PhD - Southern Connecticut State University; Amina Memon, PhD - Royal Holloway U; Rachel Zagielski, BA/BS - Southern Connecticut State University
Abstract: When and why do Innocent Suspects lie? This has never been studied, and it had always been assumed that innocent suspects were just witnesses. This is definitely not true. The current work studied innocent suspects, innocent suspects who committed a social transgression, and guilty suspects. Those who committed the social transgression often chose to omit this from their statements. This omission made them appear as guilty as the genuinely-guilty suspects. Discussion will include cases where this has happened and led to false confessions, and implications for this new paradigm of research.
References
[1]State of the States: Advancing Guilty Plea Research Through a National Survey of United States Law
Tina M. Zottoli, PhD - Montclair State University; Tarika Daftary-Kapur, PhD - Fairleigh Dickinson University; Vanessa A. Edkins, PhD - Florida Institute of Technology; Allison D. Redlich, PhD - George Mason University; Christopher M. King, JD, PhD - Montclair State University; Lucian E. Dervan, JD - Belmont University; Elizabeth Tahan, BA/BS - Montclair State University
Abstract: We conducted a national survey of statutes, case law, and court rules in the United States pertaining to the guilty plea process. We present the results of our survey in three areas (time-limited offers, collateral consequences instructions and baseless pleas) and we discuss implications for future research that is informed by jurisdictional differences in policies and procedures across the U.S. We anticipate that these data will facilitate the development of novel research questions about the impact of existing policies on plea decisionmaking and outcomes.
[2] The Order of Question Difficulty Affects Eyewitness Beliefs: A Meta-Analysis of Two Paradigms Robert B. Michael, PhD - University of Louisiana at Lafayette; Yana Weinstein, PhD - University of Massachusetts Lowell
Abstract: A simple change to test construction—flipping questions from an easy-to-hard to hard-to-easy order— affects test performance evaluations. This effect has been demonstrated in eyewitnesses and students. But with limited studies, it is difficult to know a precise estimate of the effect’s magnitude. We meta-analyzed 7,215 participants across 39 studies to more precisely estimate the influence of question difficulty order. These more precise and robust estimates show that question difficulty order affects performance evaluations: Easy-to-hard questions produce 8% more optimistic performance evaluations than hard-to-easy questions. These more precise effect size estimates have implications for test constructors and interviewers.
[3] Memory and the Cooperative Source: How Can We Obtain Richer Information?
Jacob M. Champagne, BA/BS - University of Michigan - Flint; Haley Dawson, BA/BS - University of Michigan - Flint; Peter F. Molinaro, PhD - University of Michigan - Flint
Abstract: Investigators need to gather rich information about suspects, from others, connected to that individual. While current interviewing research focuses on cooperative witnesses, little research has examined interviewing strategies for cooperative sources. Cooperative sources rely on both semantic and episodic memories when reporting their “biographical memories”. Biographical memories may be organized within autobiographical memory, and thus, be subject to organizational mnemonics. The current experiment tested two mnemonics: semantic categorization and life period retrieval strategies. Amounts of information gathered in both conditions were compared to a control condition, where no retrieval/organizational strategy was implemented. Results and implications for investigative interviewing are discussed.
[4] Emotional Memory: The Role of Personality Traits and the Central-Peripheral Trade-Off
Lauren Thompson, BA/BS - Carleton University; Ellen Tansony, MA/MS - Carleton University; Adelle Forth, PhD - Carleton University
Abstract: With a sample of 68 students, the current study sought to explore the effects of the Five-Factor Model personality dimensions and central-peripheral tradeoffs on emotional memory and attention. Participant’s eye movements were tracked as they viewed a negative, positive, and neutral image. Memory for these images was subsequently tested. Results indicated that memory was enhanced for the positive compared to the negative image and a central-peripheral trade-off was found only for the positive and neutral images. When personality 114 Saturday, March 10th was assessed, neuroticism and openness were both positively related to memory of the negative image. Eye tracking patterns were unrelated to memory.
[5] Meditative Context Reinstatement on Eyewitness Metacognition
Brittany Race, MA/MS - University of Arkansas; James M. Lampinen, PhD - University of Arkansas
Abstract: Participants watched a video featuring a car theft. They were then given a target-present or target-absent, sixperson lineup. Following their choice they were given
either confirming or neutral feedback. Following feedback participants either completed a filler task only or a filler task and a guided context reinstatement, focusing on when they made a decision in the lineup. Participants then completed post-identification feedback questions. Context reinstatement provided a more diagnostic value to confidence statements. Correct choosers reported significantly increased levels of confidence with context reinstatement, but incorrect choosers showed no significant effect of context reinstatement.
[6] Does Retrieval Enhance Eyewitness Suggestibility Because Taking an Initial Memory Test Increases the Perceived Accuracy of the Misinformation Narrative?
Krista D. Manley, MA/MS - Iowa State University; Jason CK. Chan, PhD - Iowa State University
Abstract: Despite the robust memory-enhancing benefits of retrieval practice, it can ironically exacerbate eyewitness’ susceptibility to later misinformation - a phenomenon known as Retrieval-Enhanced Suggestibility (RES; Chan, Manley, & Lang, 2017). One explanation for this finding is that after taking a memory test, participant witnesses are more likely to treat the subsequently-presented misinformation narrative as providing corrective feedback (e.g., Rindal, DeFranco, Rich, & Zaragoza, 2016). In the current study, we provided a direct test of this hypothesis by asking participants to rate their perceived accuracy of the narrative. Our results are inconsistent with this account of RES.
[7]Lies by the Innocent
Kevin Colwell, PhD - Southern Connecticut State University; Amina Memon, PhD - Royal Holloway U; Rachel Zagielski, BA/BS - Southern Connecticut State University
Abstract: When and why do Innocent Suspects lie? This has never been studied, and it had always been assumed that innocent suspects were just witnesses. This is definitely not true. The current work studied innocent suspects, innocent suspects who committed a social transgression, and guilty suspects. Those who committed the social transgression often chose to omit this from their statements. This omission made them appear as guilty as the genuinely-guilty suspects. Discussion will include cases where this has happened and led to false confessions, and implications for this new paradigm of research.
Comments
Post a Comment