Psychologists in virtually every
field have myths they have to fight. Personality psychologists have deal with
the baseless but popular Myers-Briggs personality types1,2.
Developmental psychologists have to deal with the false claim that playing
Mozart for babies improves their intelligence3. Like other deception
researchers, I have to deal with people’s persistent and inaccurate beliefs
about lies and how to catch them. I’d roughly estimate that – in my own limited
experience – somewhere around 60% of the time, when I tell someone I study
deception, they ask me if I can read people’s behavior and tell when someone is
lying. My typical answer is, “No, I can’t do that. You can’t do that. No one
can do that.”
Myths are often more appealing
than reality. They’re frequently simpler, easier to understand, and more romantic
than what science tells us4. People seem to enjoy the idea that lies
can be caught by carefully observing a person’s body language or slight
muscular movements in the face. It’s certainly a popular notion in fiction that
we need to look at the right cues in order to accurately catch lies and deceit.
The television show Lie to Me was
built on the premise of catching lies through nonverbal behavior with
astounding accuracy*. In Casino Royale,
James Bond cleverly observes Le Chiffre’s eye twitch to catch him in a poker
bluff**. In the musical Hamilton,
Angelica Schulyer points out Alexander Hamilton’s fidgety hands as he deflects
a question about his family. All this might make for entertaining fiction, but
how do popular ideas about deception stand up to scientific scrutiny? Not well.
Researchers use the term “cue to
deception” to refer to any observable behavior that might differ between
someone lying and someone telling the truth. “Observable behavior” includes
anything a human could detect without the assistance of technology. This
includes verbal behavior (what a person says), nonverbal behavior (what a
person does with their body), and paralinguistic behavior (the qualities of a
person’s speech, such as pitch or speed). When we study cues to deception, we
need to study situations in which we know for sure someone is lying or telling
the truth. For example, we might instruct people to lie or tell the truth in a
laboratory situation, or we might collect recordings of people whose lies (and
truths) were later revealed (or backed up) with incontrovertible evidence, such
as DNA testing***. Knowing who is lying and who is telling the truth is
essential because if we don’t know for sure, we won’t be able to calculate how
different lies and truths are. That is, we won’t be able to tell how well the
behavior indicates deception.
For example, if we think that liars blink more often than truth tellers (spoiler: they don’t), we will need recordings of a large number of people lying and a large number of people telling the truth. Then we need to watch all the recordings and count all the times each person blinked. We can then calculate how many times the typical liar blinks and how many times the typical truth-teller blinks and compare****. We can do this kind of counting and calculation for any kind of behavior we can see or hear.
For example, if we think that liars blink more often than truth tellers (spoiler: they don’t), we will need recordings of a large number of people lying and a large number of people telling the truth. Then we need to watch all the recordings and count all the times each person blinked. We can then calculate how many times the typical liar blinks and how many times the typical truth-teller blinks and compare****. We can do this kind of counting and calculation for any kind of behavior we can see or hear.
Despite the widespread stereotype, there's no evidence that liars don't look people in the eye. |
Thankfully, psychologists and
other researchers have been studying cues to deception for nearly a century, so
we have a huge amount of data about how people act when they lie and tell the
truth – for more than 150 different behaviors5. The data tend to
conflict with widespread beliefs about deceptive behavior. For instance, it’s a
popular stereotype that “liars can’t look you in the eye.” This belief appears
in a vast number of cultures across the world6. But there is, in
fact, no evidence that liars don’t look people in the eye.
The accumulated evidence
indicates that there indeed are some differences between truthful and deceptive
behavior. For example, liars tend to be more tense, give less detailed
descriptions, and tell less compelling stories. However, these differences are
extremely small. How small? Researchers often quantify differences by
putting them on a standardized scale, called an “effect size,” so we can
compare them more easily. One way to do this is with a measure called Cohen’s d. The typical cue to deception differs
between lies and truths with a Cohen’s d
of 0.105. This is about the same as the difference in height between
15- and 16-year-old girls7. It’s a real difference, but it’s
practically imperceptible.
Imagine you had to guess whether
a girl was age 15 or 16, based on her height and nothing else. Could you do it
accurately? You couldn’t. No matter how skilled you are, there is simply not
enough information there to make good guesses.
This is the same kind of challenge people are faced with when they try
to detect deception.
In addition to studying how liars
and truth tellers behave, for decades researchers have also been studying how
well people can catch lies. With all the data we have, psychologists have
calculated that, on average, people are about 54% accurate at distinguishing
between truths and lies8. This is pretty unimpressive. If you were
guessing using a coin flip, you’d be 50% accurate on average.
If 54% is the average, you might
be wondering if some people are better at catching lies than others. Because
the cues to deception are so weak, we wouldn’t expect there to be much
variation in people’s skill at catching lies. And that’s exactly what we see
when we calculate how much people vary in their abilities: There is virtually
no variation in lie detection ability across people9. That is,
everyone is equally bad at catching lies. However, there is a lot of variation
in people’s skill at lying. Some people are quite skilled, and it is virtually
impossible to distinguish between their lies and truths. Other people are more
transparent, and it’s easier to tell when they are lying or telling the truth.
You might be able to think of examples of times you’ve caught people lying –
but you were probably able to catch them because they told a bad lie, not
because you’re good at catching lies*****.
What about situations in which it’s
really important that we make good judgments about whether someone is lying –
when the stakes are high? Is it easier to tell if someone is lying in an
interrogation room or on the witness stand? Probably not. In fact, when you
compare accuracy rates in low-stakes and high-stakes scenarios, you see that
the accuracy rates do not change (they’re still quite low), but both liars and
truth-tellers are more likely to be judged to be lying8. That is,
everyone seems to look more suspicious when the stakes are high. Psychologists
call this “motivational impairment”10: high levels of motivation
often make people perform worse than they would otherwise, like when a star
athlete “chokes.” Liars and truth-tellers seem to be approximately equally
impaired by high levels of motivation8.
Does this mean people lie about
important things and get away with it a lot? Yes. Psychologists have conducted
studies in which they ask people to report lies they’ve told. The participants
are promised complete anonymity, to encourage them to be honest about their
past deceit. In a study in which people were asked to describe serious lies
they had told – including criminal behavior and marital infidelity – about 40%
of the lies were never discovered11. The vast majority of the lies
people tell are small and harmless – intended to spare someone’s feelings or to
keep relationships running smoothly. Some lies are serious and potentially
harmful. But we are terrible at detecting the harmless ones and the serious
ones alike.
The collected findings of the
science of deception have serious implications. An important lesson to take
from deception research is that we should be skeptical when we hear someone
claiming they have a way to accurately catch lies. There’s no shortage of
books, videos, and training packages that allege they can help you detect
deception or read body language. Many (if not most) of these are not based on
any scientific evidence. People accumulating fame and money from selling these
products might not care12, 13.
Another important lesson to take
from research is that we must be highly cautious about the training we invest
in and the policies we enact as they pertain to deception and deception
judgments. In legal contexts, correctly judging whether someone is lying or
telling the truth may have consequences for people’s lives and liberty:
Mistaking a true denial of a crime for a lie can lead to a wrongful conviction;
mistaking a false denial for an honest denial can allow a perpetrator to elude
justice.
Transportation security is one domain in which the science of deception has been mishandled. |
If we don’t use what we know
about deception, it can also be highly wasteful. Governments and law
enforcement agencies have not always made decisions consistent with the science
of deception. For example, the Transportation Security Administration (TSA)
spent nearly $900,000,000 on a program called Screening Passengers by
Observational Techniques (SPOT) – a package of training and security measures
largely based on the premise of visually examining airline passengers for
behavioral cues to deception14. The program was intended to help identify
potential threats to transportation security. The techniques used in this
program were inconsistent with the research on deception, and there was no
scientific reason to think it would work. It was likely a colossal waste of
resources.
Catching lies with unerring
accuracy might be fun to watch in movies or on TV. But it’s wasteful, if not
dangerous, to mistake myths and fantasy for reality. These fictional portrayals
of deception detection are just that – fictional.
Timothy Luke wrote and Will
Crozier edited this post
Notes
* For some time, Lie to Me claimed to be based on
science, but this assertion was later removed from the promotional materials. Ironically,
exposure to Lie to Me might actually
make people worse at catching lies. See Levine, T. R., Serota, K. B., &
Shulman, H. C. (2010). The impact of Lie to Me on viewers’ actual ability to
detect deception. Communication Research, 37, 847-856.
** Of course, this ends up
spectacularly backfiring when Le Chiffre learns that Bond has discovered his
“tell.”
*** For example, some researchers
have examined video recordings of people pleading to the media for the public
to provide information about a relatively who had gone missing. In some of the
cases, it was later proven that the pleader in fact killed their relative. In
other cases, it was proven they were innocent.
**** Point of interest: One of my
first research jobs was counting the number of times people blinked in interrogations.
It was one of the most mind-numbing things I’ve done in my life.
***** By definition, any time you
fail to catch someone lying to you, you don’t know it happened. For that
reason, people often have an inflated sense of how good they are at catching
lies: You can remember the times you caught lies successfully (which can give
you the impression you’re good at it), but you simply have no knowledge of all
the times you’ve been duped (and it could be a lot).
References
[1] Pittenger, D. J. (2005). Cautionary
Comments Regarding the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Consulting
Psychology Journal: Practice and Research, 57, 210-221.
[2]
https://www.vox.com/2014/7/15/5881947/myers-briggs-personality-test-meaningless
[3] Pietschnig, J., Voracek, M., & Formann, A. K. (2010).
Mozart effect–Shmozart effect: A meta-analysis. Intelligence, 38,
314-323.
[4] Lewandowsky, S., Ecker, U. K., Seifert, C.
M., Schwarz, N., & Cook, J. (2012). Misinformation and its correction:
Continued influence and successful debiasing. Psychological Science in
the Public Interest, 13, 106-131.
[5] DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. J., Malone, B.
E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K., & Cooper, H. (2003). Cues to
Deception. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 74-118.
[6] Global Deception Research Team. (2006). A
world of lies. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 37,
60-74.
[7] https://www.cdc.gov/growthcharts/data/set2clinical/cj41c072.pdf
[8] Bond Jr, C. F., & DePaulo, B. M.
(2006). Accuracy of deception judgments. Personality and Social
Psychology Review, 10, 214-234.
[9] Bond Jr, C. F., & DePaulo, B. M.
(2008). Individual Differences in Judging Deception: Accuracy and Bias. Psychological
Bulletin, 134, 477-492.
[10] DePaulo, B. M., Kirkendol, S. E., Tang, J., & O'Brien, T.
P. (1988). The motivational impairment effect in the communication of
deception: Replications and extensions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 12,
177-202.
[11] DePaulo, B. M., Ansfield, M. E.,
Kirkendol, S. E., & Boden, J. M. (2004). Serious lies. Basic and
Applied Social Psychology, 26, 147-167.
[12] Eriksson, A., & Lacerda, F. (2007).
Charlatanry in forensic speech science: A problem to be taken seriously. International
Journal of Speech, Language and the Law, 14, 169-193.
[13] Masip, J., Herrero, C., Garrido, E., &
Barba, A. (2011). Is the behaviour analysis interview just common sense? Applied
Cognitive Psychology, 25, 593-604
[14] Government Accountability Office (2013). TSA Should Limit Future Funding for Behavior
Detection Activities. GAO-14-159. https://www.gao.gov/assets/660/658923.pdf
Comments
Post a Comment