Authority plays a prominent – perhaps obvious – role in the
legal system. Police, lawyers, and judges all wield prestige and authority. Psychologists
have been interested in people’s tendency to obey authority for decades, and much
of our understanding of the psychology of authority is built on the work of one
man. If you’ve taken an introductory psychology class, you have probably heard
about Stanley Milgram’s studies on obedience to authority. Milgram’s program of
obedience research is some of the most famous – if not the most famous – work in psychology. Many people have heard of
Milgram’s work, but most people don’t know the details of his experiments or
about the follow-up work of researchers who came after him.
In the most well-known version of Milgram’s obedience
experiment procedure1, an experimenter first greets two participants
and explains that they are to take part in a study on the role of punishment in
learning. The experimenter (apparently) randomly assigns one of the
participants to play the roles of “teacher” and “learner.” However, one of the
participants is actually a member of the research team (what we call a
“confederate”). The assignment procedure is rigged so the confederate is always
assigned to be the learner. The learner is brought to a separate room (equipped
with a microphone and speaker so the teacher and learner can communicate) and
hooked up to an electric shock generator. It is then the teacher’s task to
guide the learner through a series of memorization tasks, and each time the
learner fails to correctly answer a question, the teacher must administer an
electrical shock to the learner, with increasing voltage each time. The maximum
voltage switch was labeled with a vague but ominous “XXX.” With each shock, the
learner cried out in pain and obviously increasing distress. The experimenter
does relatively little in this procedure, but if the teacher expresses
reluctance to shock the learner, he has a series of scripted verbal prods that
request that the teacher continue to administer the shocks. For example, the
teacher is told, “The experiment requires you to continue.”
A diagram of the typical obedience experiment. |
The results were striking: More than 60% of the participants
proceed to administer shocks to the learner up to the maximum voltage, despite
his vocal objections. Milgram conducted studies with 23 variations on the
procedures, and each variation produced different levels of obedience2.
For instance, in some variations of the procedure, the learner claimed to have
a heart condition and, after receiving several shocks, simply stopped
responding (having apparently collapsed and possibly died). Despite the
apparent gravity of the situation, this addition did not change the rates of
obedience very much. One version, in which the teacher was only indirectly
responsible for the shocks (the teacher read out the questions, and another
person gave the shocks), elevated obedience rates to staggeringly high levels
of more than 90%. There were two variations in particular that produced notably
low rates of obedience: (1) when the experimenter did not directly instruct the
participant to increase the shocks and (2) when the learner was a friend or
relative of the teacher. However, obedience rates were not zero in these
versions; they were around 12-15% – remarkably high considering the possibly
fatal consequences if the procedures were real.
There are times in Milgram’s writing that he seems deeply
unsettled by his results – that under circumstances that should intuitively
have restrained people from obeying immoral commands, people continued to
administer shocks. In 1965, he wrote,
What is the limit of such
obedience? At many points we attempted to establish a boundary. Cries from the
victim were inserted; not good enough. The victim claimed heart trouble;
subjects still shocked him on command. The victim pleaded that he be let free,
and his answers no longer registered on the signal box; subjects continued to
shock him. At the outset we had not conceived that such drastic procedures
would be needed to generate disobedience, and each step was added only as the
ineffectiveness of the earlier techniques became clear. The final effort to
establish a limit was the Touch-Proximity condition [in which the teacher had
to physically hold learner against the shock generator]. But the very first
subject in this condition subdued the victim on command, and proceeded to the
highest shock level. A quarter of the subjects in this condition performed
similarly. The results, as seen and felt in the laboratory, are to this author
disturbing3.
As compelling and disturbing as Milgram’s results are, there
is some disagreement among psychologists about how exactly to interpret them.
There are numerous challenges for researchers trying to explain why the rates
of obedience vary under different circumstances. First, Milgram did not seem to
have a clearly charted plan for the different variations of the procedure that
he tested. That is, he seemed to be testing different situations that he
intuitively thought might influence the rates of disobedience, rather than
sequentially testing a particular theory. Second, the standards of research
ethics have changed since Milgram’s time. People are understandably concerned
about the distress participants experience when they believe they have hurt
someone during the experiment and, after the experiment is done, knowing that
they are capable of hurting and possibly killing someone. Because of this, it
is challenging for researchers to use variations of Milgram’s procedures today.
Some researchers have replicated the obedience experiment with modified
protocols that stop the procedure early, at around the middle of the voltage
scale, sparing participants the possibility of thinking they have killed
someone. Although these replications have been informative (and have largely
obtained results very similar to what Milgram found4), they remove
what is arguably the most interesting part of the study. Because Milgram’s
original data are somewhat limited and because ethical concerns restrict our
ability to conduct follow-up studies, we can’t easily collect the data we would
probably need to put the settle arguments of interpretation. However, we’ve
still learned a lot from Milgram’s work and from the efforts to make sense of
it.
One of the most interesting and prominent explanations for
Milgram’s findings is that rather than blindly following authority, people are
motivated to comply by identifying with the experimenter, and they are
motivated to disobey by identifying with the learner5. That is,
people in an obedience study have two competing sources of responsibility. They
are ostensibly a critical part of a legitimate scientific study, but they have
a responsibility not to unduly harm the learner. Under circumstances that lead
them to identify more with the experimenter – for example, when the experiment
takes place under the auspices of a prestigious university or when they are
relatively detached from shocking the learner – they obey at high rates. Under
circumstances that lead them to identify more with the learner – for example
when they have a prior relationship with the learner or when they must
administer the shocks with their own hands – they are less likely to obey.
Another way of thinking about this is that people are more likely to do things
they believe are wrong if they think they are doing them for a purpose they
also believe in and feel close to.
These tendencies toward obedience can have devastating
results. People “following orders” have committed atrocities such as the My Lai
massacre or the liquidation of the gulags6. Milgram himself set out
to study obedience because he was interested in explaining how so many ordinary
people could have helped carry out the Holocaust. His writing makes his
concerns about the perils of obedience quite clear: “If… an anonymous
experimenter could successfully command adults to subdue a fifty-year-old man,
and force on him painful electric shocks against his protests, one can only
wonder what government, with its vastly greater authority and prestige, can
command of its subjects.”3
Obedience may have more subtle, smaller-scale (but still
grave) consequences, too. To our knowledge, there is no experimental research
on these issues, but close examination of wrongful conviction cases suggests
that obedience may play a role in some false confessions. Many proven false
confessors were under the impression they were serving an important role and
assisting the police. Sometimes, this impression was formed because of lies
told by interrogators. For example, Adrian
Thomas, who falsely admitted to murdering an infant, was told if he could
describe exactly how he caused the injuries, doctors might be able to save the
child’s life*. Thomas provided a video-recorded demonstration of how he hurt the
infant – even though he hadn’t actually done it. Exonerees Jeff Descovic
and Amanda Knox have also described how they were led to believe they were
important sources of information for murder investigations. Both of them
ultimately provided false incriminating statements to the police. For at least
part of these investigations, all three of these exonerees seemed to believe
they were assisting in a legitimate and just cause by bending to the authority
of interrogators. This is admittedly speculation, but it’s possible that the
willingness to confession – even to things you didn’t do – is increased by the
tendency to obey authorities whose motives you believe in and with whom you
identify.
Milgram’s work shone a light on a dark and troubling part of
human behavior. Although it is decades old, the research continues to be
relevant, to inform researchers, and to help us understand puzzling behavior. Research
on obedience to authority is difficult to do, but given the damage obedience can
cause, it is well worth the effort.
The post was written by Timothy Luke and edited by Will
Crozier.
Notes
* The infant was, in fact, already dead when the
interrogators told Thomas his confession was so urgently required. It was later
found that the child had not been injured and had actually died of a separate
medical issue.
References
[1] Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience
to authority: An experimental view. New York: Harper & Row.
[1] Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,
67, 371-378.
[2] Haslam, N., Loughnan, S., & Perry, G. (2014).
Meta-Milgram: An empirical synthesis of the obedience experiments. PloS
one, 9(4), e93927.
[3] Milgram, S. (1965). Some conditions of obedience and
disobedience to authority. Human Relations, 18(1),
57-76.
[4] Burger, J. M. (2009). Replicating Milgram: Would people
still obey today? American Psychologist, 64(1), 1-11.
[4] Doliński, D., Grzyb, T., Folwarczny, M., Grzybała, P.,
Krzyszycha, K., Martynowska, K., & Trojanowski, J. (2017). Would you
deliver an electric shock in 2015? Obedience in the experimental paradigm
developed by Stanley Milgram in the 50 years following the original studies. Social
Psychological and Personality Science, 8(8), 927-933.
[4] Dolinski, D., & Grzyb, T. (2016). One Serious Shock
Versus Gradated Series of Shocks: Does “Multiple Feet-in-the-Door” Explain
Obedience in Milgram Studies? Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 38(5),
276-283.
[5] Reicher, S. D., Haslam, S. A., & Smith, J. R.
(2012). Working toward the experimenter: Reconceptualizing obedience within the
Milgram paradigm as identification-based followership. Perspectives on
Psychological Science, 7(4), 315-324.
[5] Haslam, S. A., Reicher, S. D., & Birney, M. E.
(2014). Nothing by mere authority: Evidence that in an experimental analogue of
the Milgram paradigm participants are motivated not by orders but by appeals to
science. Journal of Social Issues, 70(3), 473-488.
[6] Glover, J. (1999). Humanity:
A moral history of the twentieth century. Yale University Press.
You are providing good knowledge. It is really helpful and factual information for us and everyone to increase knowledge. Continue sharing your data about psychologist stouffville. Thank you.
ReplyDeleteYou can have all of that right here with this video slot - your max bet can go all finest way|the 카지노 사이트 method in which} a lot as} $1000, and that claims lots about how brave of a spirit you need to|you should|you have to} be. Helen, aka probably the most stunning woman on the earth, will gift you three re-spins to pile up as many high-paying symbols as you can to|you probably can}. Ivory Citadel is themed around an ancient, mystical jungle temple. The animals rule this specific jungle properly as|in addition to} its bonus rounds, and also you won’t catch sight of any humans. Casual playing lovers to excessive rollers, with bets starting from $0.6 to $600, Fortune Coin manages to keep the gaming expertise of each camps a top-notch one.
ReplyDelete